The MIT Rape Study and Other Sloppy Surveys

The latest alarming numbers on campus sexual assault come from a Massachusetts Institute of Technology survey which supposedly shows that 17 percent of female undergraduates have been sexually assaulted during their time at the school. Writing in The Washington Post, columnist Catherine Rampell has invoked these findings as a rebuke to those who have criticized earlier studies for inflating campus sexual assault statistics. But, in fact, the MIT survey starkly illustrates the very problems those critics have pointed out.

For one, there is the issue of response bias, acknowledged at the end of survey itself: only 35 percent of the MIT students who received the survey answered it, and it’s entirely possible that people who have had unwanted sexual experiences were more likely to respond. But, more importantly, there is also a question of how sexual assault is defined.

The sexual assault data were derived from two questions on the survey:

Have you experienced any of the following while at MIT?

 Someone fondled, kissed, or rubbed up against the private areas of my body or removed some of my clothes even though I didn’t want to

Someone TRIED to sexually penetrate me (put a penis or insert fingers or objects into my vagina or anus) even though I didn’t want to

Someone sexually penetrated me even though I didn’t want to

Someone TRIED to perform oral sex on me or make me give them oral sex even though I didn’t want to

Someone performed oral sex on me or made me give them oral sex even  though I didn’t want to

 Did the person or persons who did one or more of the behaviors listed above do them by…

Catching you off guard, or ignoring non-verbal cues or looks?

Telling lies, threatening to end the relationship or to spread rumors about you, or verbally pressuring you?

Showing displeasure, criticizing your sexuality or attractiveness, or getting angry?

Taking advantage of you when you were too drunk, high, asleep or out of it?

Threatening to physically harm you or someone close to you?

Using force, or having a weapon?

Those who answered in the affirmative and picked one or more of the last three options—incapacitation, threat of physical harm, or force—were counted as victims of sexual assault: 17 percent of female undergraduates, 5 percent of male undergraduates, 5 percent of female graduate students, and 1 percent of male graduate students. But what was the breakdown for sexual assault by incapacitation versus physical force or threat? Here, the MIT report refers to “respondents who indicated that one or more of these experiences involved the use of force or a weapon (5%), threat of physical harm (1%), or incapacitation (being taken advantage of while too drunk, high, asleep or out of it – 44%).” These percentages sound confusing at first; I was finally able to puzzle out that they refer to the entire pool of students who reported some form of unwanted sexual behavior, including conduct not classified as sexual assault such as verbal or emotional pressure.

If you do the math, it turns out that among students whose unwanted experiences were counted as sexual assault, only 10 percent reported being physically forced or threatened with harm. (Assuming this pattern is roughly the same for all subgroups, that adds up to fewer than 2 percent of undergraduate women.) The other 90 percent had been “taken advantage of” while incapacitated—with incapacitation defined rather subjectively as being “too drunk [or] high.” It is simply impossible to tell how many of these incidents involved intoxication so severe as to meet the legal criteria for lacking the capacity to consent.

Complicating things further, the survey’s definition of “sexual assault” included the fairly broad category of “sexual touching or kissing,” as well as attempted oral sex or sexual penetration. (Could this include a situation in which one person makes an overly aggressive move but stops and apologizes when the other person says no?) Six percent of undergraduate women reported unwanted penetration due to force or incapacitation, while three percent reported coerced oral sex; but the study gives no data on how many of these cases involved actual incapacitation, rather than alcohol-impaired judgment.

In a separate survey item, the students were directly asked whether they had been sexually assaulted or raped while attending MIT. One in ten undergraduate women, and 2 percent of the men, reported being sexually assaulted, while 5 percent of the women and 1 percent of the men reported being raped. Altogether, 11 percent of female undergraduates answered “yes” on one or both of those questions.

Clearly, many young women who reported an experience included in the study’s sexual assault statistics did not think they had been raped or sexually assaulted. That discrepancy between 17 percent and 11 percent is the focus of Rampell’s Washington Post piece. According to Rampell, “One of the frequent complaints levied against college sexual assault statistics is that they are improbably high, presumably because survey respondents take too broad a view of what counts as ‘sexual assault.’” The MIT study, she asserts, shows the opposite: too often, young people “don’t recognize that many of the unwanted sexual behaviors they’ve experienced meet the definition of ‘sexual assault.’”

Actually, Rampell’s column shows that she has very little knowledge of the critiques she is attempting to refute. The fact that campus sexual assault surveys label women as rape victims even when those women do not believe they were raped is a main point in these critiques, going all the way back to the work of University of California-Berkeley sociologist Neil Gilbert and “dissident feminist” scholar Christina Hoff Sommers in the 1990s.

And, as we have seen, the MIT survey provides ample grist for these critiques, especially when one looks at its other findings. For instance: while fewer then 5 percent of the students who had experienced unwanted sexual behavior reported it to someone in an official capacity, the main reason for not reporting the incident (cited by 72 percent) was that it wasn’t “serious enough to officially report.”  Also, “55 percent indicated it was not clear harm was intended”; “47 percent did not want any action to be taken”; and “44 percent felt that they were at least partly at fault or it wasn’t totally the other person’s fault.”

Yet Rampell is not the only commentator to argue that young women’s failure to label their experiences as rape or sexual assault is a problem. Time’s Eliza Gray writes that “a deeper look at the numbers points to a more troubling statistic”:

Even though 17% of female undergraduates reported an experience that fits the survey’s definition of sexual assault … only 11% of female undergraduates checked “yes” when asked directly if they had been “raped” or “sexually assaulted.” Despite a concerted effort by the Obama Administration, state officials and campus leaders, MIT students were uncertain about what qualified as sexual violence — even when reporting that they had experienced assault.

On the other hand, in a rare dissenting voice in The New Republic, New York-based writer Batya Ungar-Sargon argues that “MIT’s—and the media’s—preference for the 17-percent figure suggests it doesn’t trust students to properly classify their own sexual encounters.” In Ungar-Sargon’s view, to discount the fact that only 10 percent of female undergrads at MIT actually say they have been sexually assaulted “is to remove the students’ very canny ability to distinguish the criminal from the unwanted.”

Ungar-Sargon makes many excellent points about the shoddiness of the MIT survey (though, because of the confusing way in which the MIT report handles the numbers, she actually underestimates the percentage of “sexual assaults” related to alcohol or drugs).  But in the current campus climate, I’m not sure the students’ own labeling of their experiences can be considered reliable—given the strenuous effort to persuade young women to lump the unwanted with the criminal. It is quite likely, for instance, that many MIT students today think all drunken sexual contact is rape, at least if you feel that you wouldn’t have consented to it in a sober state.  Indeed, it is entirely possible that some students who were not counted as sexual assault victims in the MIT survey—ones who had yielded to “verbal pressure,” for example—were among the 10 percent who believed they had been sexually assaulted.

Indeed, one remarkable item in the MIT survey shows just how confused campus discourse on sexual assault has become. More than half of all students, and two-thirds of undergrads—women and men alike—agreed that “rape and sexual assault can happen unintentionally, especially if alcohol is involved.” Yet, tellingly, only 13 percent agreed that “when someone is raped or sexually assaulted, it’s often because the way they said ‘no’ was unclear or there was some miscommunication,” while only 20 percent agreed that a victim who is drunk “is at least somewhat responsible for putting themselves in that position.” In other words, most students believe that unwanted sex due to miscommunication or alcohol-addled judgment amounts to “unintentional” rape—but only a minority are willing to place responsibility on both parties.

The depressing truth is that campus sexual assault surveys have become virtually meaningless. That will not change until we stop and reverse the definitional drift in discussions of sexual assault.

Author

13 thoughts on “The MIT Rape Study and Other Sloppy Surveys

  1. I have a simple way to get a reliable survey, where all would agree. Before the survey is done, its questions and methodology should be reviewed by conservative scholars who have criticised past surveys, and only proceed with the survey when all agree the methodology is valid, and the results could not be criticised. This way, biased questions with overly broad definitions would be gone, and it would be decided in advance what to do with no responses (I would treat them as negative, on the assumption that any woman with something to report in the survey would be likely to report it, while those with nothing to report would be more likely to ignore it).
    Unfortunately, the leftist scholars running these surveys probably would never do this, because they dont want honest surveys that all can agree on, they want biased surveys that “prove” what they want to prove.

  2. “The other 90 percent had been “taken advantage of” while incapacitated—with incapacitation defined rather subjectively as being “too drunk [or] high.” It is simply impossible to tell how many of these incidents involved intoxication so severe as to meet the legal criteria for lacking the capacity to consent.”

    An Occidental college student was kicked out when a woman complained a week after having sex with him. She had texted a friend “I’m going to have sex now” – but was deemed to drunk to actually give consent.

    Basically – there is no standard- if a woman drinks- she can later claim she didn’t give sober consent. The man has to figure this out for her- and his drinking is no excuse, of course.

  3. “Someone TRIED to sexually penetrate me (put a penis or insert fingers or objects into my vagina or anus) even though I didn’t want to”

    Well- that’s a problem- it sounds sinister until you envision the average hot and heavy make-out session. Two people are in bed together, at some point a man puts his hand to “get to third base”. She slaps his hand away. They go back to making out- “second base”. They might have penetration/full sex the next day, or next week.

    This happens every day- certainly every weekend.

    There you go- America has a rape culture on par with India or South Africa.

    1. The “even though I didn’t want to” doesn’t mean “I didn’t want to right that second” it means “I didn’t want to.” As in the person was never going to get there in the first place without trying to force it in.

    1. Reports from healthcare professionals are reliable, but that doesn’t necessarily mean things that are unreported to professionals or reported to different professionals besides the healthcare industry are unreliable.

  4. Re: “only 35 percent of the MIT students who received the survey answered it, and it’s entirely possible that people who have had unwanted sexual experiences were more likely to respond.”

    True, but… in the footnotes we learn that 46% of undergrad women responded. If *all* female undergrad non-respondents had instead reported no sexual assaults, that would roughly halve the relevant figures, taking the reported assault rate down to about 5%. That is not 20% but is is still high.

    And another problem is that the classic ‘1 in 5’ stat (the many debunkings of which I heartily endorse) refers to a woman’s four year college experience. The MIT survey, taken in late spring, is a mix of freshmen, sophomores, juniors and seniors.

    If, based on zero evidence, we assume that all four classes had an equal size and response rate and that assault is equally likely in all four years, the 5% figure would need to be adjusted upward to reflect the fact that the ‘average’ respondent had only been on campus 2.5 years, rather than 4. My cursory arithmetic suggests that a 5% overall rate across four classes is consistent with roughly 9% of seniors having been assaulted at some time. Again, well less than 20%, but not negligible, either.

    It does seem that MIT could make a tiny effort and release the cross-tabs for the survey to resolve some of these points.

    1. 17% out of 46% weighted with 0% out of the remaining 54% is about 7.8%, so I don’t know where you got 5% from. It would be nice if they broke it down by class year though.

  5. Sorta like how the global warming hockey stick paper was “confirmed” by additional studies that relied on the same exact flawed methodology.

  6. It looks like a combination of self selection bias and very wishy washy definitions. It also fails to account for what percentage of these sexual assaults were actually perpetrated by fellow students.

    Beyond all these, what good is a study that counts a gunpoint forcible rape the same as a drunken groping? To me, it’s clearly twisting statistics to fit the narrative – it’s like political pollsters twisting the questions to get the answers they want.

    1. This study does not count a gunpoint forcible rape as the same as a drunken groping. However, in the statement “17% of female students are victims of sexual assault or rape” then both drunken gropings and gunpoint rape are in that 17%. Nowhere does it say “these are the same thing.”
      The definitions are also not “wishy-washy,” they are very clear. However the problem arises when the terms used within the definitions are very subjective, such as the definition of “Tried” as in “someone tried to penetrate you.” That could mean “asked and got a no response” or “went full on penetration until forced to stop.”

  7. As usual Cathy Young does an exemplary job of analysis. But, it’s like closing the stable door after the frisky little mare has bolted!

    The abuse of Rape Statistics has been ongoing since the 1970’s, took of apace under the wings of St Steinem of Ms Magazine and her infamous 1988 Rape report. The woozles are legion, and the Woozle Effect has been in full play with citation replacing evidence.

    Rape and Rape culture are the bread and butter of the Feminist Industries, just as GM is the bread and butter of Agribuisness. You will never get Feminists to give up cooking the rape culture statistical books, so don’t bother.

    Many wonder why anyone would deliberately warp stats and reality. For feminists it’s summed up as “Carceral Feminism”: a distinct logic within a broader set of liberal feminist strategies focusing on a law and order agenda. If in doubt demand incarceration, police and state intervention and all forms of special protection that can be facilitated from the state – and all biased on grounds of sex. Throw in family courts and custody and you have a Tsunami of Incarceration and Control in the name of Feminism.

    By having behaviours redefined as socially unacceptable, pathologised and made to verge on the criminality, it allows for extensive pressure to be brought to bear upon legislatures to change all social boundaries in one desired direction. IN the US VAWA is a prime example. It should address matters in equality, but the bias is up front in the name.

    The MIT questions are framed to exclude male victims, as rape by envelopment is not addressed. It fails to address patterns of coercion where often female coercion of the male becomes apparent POST event. There is s a great deal of gender bias that serves only the “Carceral Feminist” ideal and which ignores the rights of all people, irrespective of sex or genitals, to be treated fairly and in equality in sight of The Law. It’s already quite impossible for that to occur across most western societies as the negative views of men needed to empower “Carceral Feminist” ideas have already been established and are being exploited.

    The area of battle and defence is the Higher Courts and Protection in sight of The Law.

    1. As usual, Cathy Young pushes an agenda with the same dishonesty as the feminists/politicians pushing “1 in 5” statistics.
      Some especially turd-like analysis from Young:
      “The other 90 percent had been “taken advantage of” while incapacitated—with incapacitation defined rather subjectively as being “too drunk [or] high.” It is simply impossible to tell how many of these incidents involved intoxication so severe as to meet the legal criteria for lacking the capacity to consent.”

      Young is doing two things here:
      1. Implying that added violence somehow validates calling it sexual assault. In fact the main reason why such a large number of sexual assaults involve alcohol or other drugs is precisely because of the “stereotypical” violent rape. Because they think they have a higher chance of getting away with it if they just knock the victim out beforehand. “How could it be a rape if I didn’t force her down?”
      2. Trying to bait the reader with the “it’s impossible to tell how many of them were legally incapacitated” garbage. She wants readers to think that a bunch of people are trying to exaggerate their intoxication level so that it could be classified as assault. Which makes absolutely no sense in this case because there’s no incentive for people to try and play victim on an anonymous survey that they gain absolutely nothing from (and lose precious study time filling out the survey).

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *